Research Article - Journal of Pain Management and Therapy (2018) Volume 2, Issue 1
Is there a correlation between objective and subjective pain measurements and gamma oscillation frequencies?
Philip Bright*, Samantha NottageResearch Department, The European School of Osteopathy, Maidstone, United Kingdom
- *Corresponding Author:
- Philip Bright
Research Department, The European School of Osteopathy, Boxley
Kent, United Kingdom
Tel: +44 1622 671 558
E-mail: philbright@eso.co.uk
Accepted date: January 03, 2018
Citation: Bright P, Nottage S. Is there a correlation between objective and subjective pain measurements and gamma oscillation frequencies? J Pain Manage Ther. 2018;2(1):1-7.
Abstract
Pain is a primal neurological and psychological function supporting self- preservation but prone to aberrant processing. Objective and subjective pain measures exist which are regarded as key outcomes with significant clinical indicators of change. The relationships between these measures warrant further exploration. The objective of this observational study was to explore potential correlation between objective and subjective pain measurements and gamma oscillation frequencies. Baseline characteristics were collected and brainwave activities were recorded via an EEG device for 37 undergraduate osteopathy students in a convenience sample. Pain pressure threshold was recorded via an algometer and participants were asked to complete a numeric rating scale (NRS) for pain. Multiple regression tests were analysed for correlation between the participant baseline variables (age, gender, anxiety levels, height and weight). High gamma oscillation, PPT and NRS were seen to have a significant correlation (p>0.05, r²=0.911 (95% Confidence Interval 0.83 - 0.96)). Exercises targeting modulation of gamma oscillations may play an important role in pain control. Whilst findings are significant, the generalisation of the results in this study is limited and further research into gamma oscillations in chronic pain populations is warranted.
Perspective: This article explores the relationship between pain measures and gamma oscillation frequencies. This relationship could potentially assist in pain control through directly targeting gamma waves through gaming activities.
Keywords
Pain pressure threshold, gamma oscillations, numeric rating scale.
Introduction
Pain is a common experience and a highly subjective sensation yet the neural mechanisms underlying pain perception are not well understood [1,2]. There have been studies suggesting a link between gamma oscillations and pain perception [3,4] but limited research links gamma oscillations to pain pressure threshold. Pain has obvious sensory qualities but it can also be subject to psychological expectations and state of mind [1].
The International Association for the Study of Pain considers the sensation as: 'An unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage or describes in terms of such damage' [5]. This indicates that an individual’s experience of pain is subjective and depends on a complex integration of not only emotional factors but possible physiological damage. Consequently, multifactorial modalities are implicated in the treatment of pain.
A painful stimulus is a warning of paramount behavioural relevance within a survival context. It is likely that relevant brain regions will receive heightened processing when a painful stimulus is experienced [6]. Several studies appear to suggest gamma oscillations have a close link to pain perception. Nociceptive stimuli have been seen to provoke high-frequency oscillation activity of the human primary somatosensory cortex and gamma oscillations. Sensory representation of a painful stimulus and its selection for further processing may be activated by pain induced gamma oscillations [7].
It has been hypothesised that gamma oscillations may be involved in encoding pain intensities. Gamma oscillations can be considered as markers of motor behaviour preparation in relation to pain or subjective pain perception processes [8,9]. Pain-induced gamma oscillations may represent a marker of sensory processing whose influence on pain perception differs with behavioural context [10].
Neuronal changes in the human brain illustrate a close link to the variable attentional effect of pain [11]. Gamma oscillations appear to be integral to visual attention [12]. Visual evoked gamma oscillations in the form of light therapy were presented to migraine sufferers. After 30 days of gamma stimulation, migraine sufferers reported a 53% decrease in migraine frequency, suggesting a role in gamma oscillations and modulating pain [13].
Gamma oscillations have been suggestive to be indicative of conscious pain perception and consequently could be used to assess chronic pain patients [9]. A study on rats suggested that during late stages of chronic inflammatory conditions gamma oscillations increased considerably. This indicates that elevated gamma oscillations may be a potential biomarker for chronic pain [14]. EEG signals were observed during tonic muscle pain in humans which also detected enhanced levels of gamma oscillations parallel to pain intensities [15].
Mindfulness is a therapeutic tool now being offered to people in chronic pain, it has been implicated that there is a significant correlation between mindfulness and high gamma oscillations [16]. Gamma oscillations in musicians significantly increased whilst listening to music, perhaps indicating gamma oscillations play a role in the neural mechanism with the use of music therapy [17]. Musical beats were also shown to induce gamma oscillations in non-musicians [18]. The data suggests that cognitive engagement may induce long- term and short term neural change and modulation in which gamma oscillations potentially lay a role [19].
This relationship between pain and gamma-band activity may modulate painful stimuli by top down influences such as attention [20]. Although certain activities are shown to increase gamma oscillations their role in pain is largely unknown. The link between gamma oscillations and subjective pain measurements has been studied; the link between gamma oscillations and objective pain measures, such as pain pressure threshold (PPT), has not been fully explored. Demonstrating a link between objective pain and gamma oscillations could be fundamental to people suffering from chronic pain and may enable practitioners to give exercises targeting gamma oscillation to mitigate analgesic effects.
Aim and Objectives
The aim of this study was to answer the research question: What is the relationship between objective and subjective pain measurements and gamma oscillation frequencies? The objective was to establish whether correlation exists between high gamma oscillation frequencies, numeric rating scale (NRS) for pain and pain pressure threshold (PPT).
Methods
Design
Cross-sectional observational study.
Participants
This study recruited participants from student cohorts attending a UK Osteopathic Educational Institution that met the study criteria.
Inclusion criteria
Asymptomatic healthy subjects between the ages of 18 and 59.
Exclusion criteria
Participants suffering any conditions affecting the nervous system, musculoskeletal system or any condition that may affect pain perception.
Outcome measures
To provide the pain pressure threshold the Wagner Force Dial™ FDK 40 with a 1cm rubber tipped probe was used. These algometry values were recorded in kg/cm² (Wagner Instruments 2014). Gamma oscillation frequencies were monitored using a Myndplay headband with accompanying software (MyndPlay version 2013 2.3.0pro).
Procedure
The piloted study was conducted at the Teaching Clinic of the European School of Osteopathy between June and September 2015; the same treatment room was used each time for consistency. Information sheets, consent forms and questionnaires were dispensed and participants were asked to give a subjective pain measurement using a numerical rating scale (NRS) which rated from 0 to 10 (0 represented ‘no pain’ and 10 represented “worst pain imaginable”) [21].
Participants were then blindfolded while lying in a supine position on a clinic couch. Verbal communication was not permitted during the data gathering process outside of initial instruction.
The EEG Myndplay headband was then used to record neural activity via three electrodes; one clipped to the participant’s ear lobe, the following two placed onto the forehead. The data was recorded onto a laptop PC while the participant lay still for 10 minutes. The mean scores of the gamma oscillations were calculated for 10 minute periods.
Once the readings were recorded and EEG band was removed, the Force Dial was then used to identify an objective PPT score. Participants were asked to say stop when they began to experience discomfort. Measurements were taken from the right thenar eminence, common extensor tendon of the forearm and the levator scapula muscle. The mean of the three PPT readings were then calculated and used as the individuals PPT score.
Ethics approval
This study was conducted in compliance with the principles of the declaration of Helsinki (1996) and all of the applicable regulatory requirements. The protocol was approved by the European School of Osteopathy Research Ethics Committee.
Data analysis
Summary and inferential statistics were calculated using Excel version 14 (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA) and Analyse-it version 3.76 (Analyse-it Software, Ltd., Leeds, UK). Assumptions for parametric tests were explored and relationships between variables were tested using Spearman’s Correlation and a multiple regression approach; the predictors were PPT, subjective pain measured by NRS, age, anxiety levels height and weight. Differences in gender response were further examined using Mann-Whitney U. The significance level for all statistical tests was set to 5% with confidence intervals of 95%.
Results
Thirty four healthy participants, aged between 18–59 years, provided data for analysis. The baseline characteristics and data collection can be seen in Table 1.
Characteristics | Mean(SD) or Ratio |
---|---|
Gamma | 7469.65 (8693.90) |
PPT | 4.24 (1.56) |
Age | 29 (9.97) |
Anxiety levels (0-10) | 3.94 (2.36) |
Meditation frequency ratio (yes/ no / occasionally) | 3: 24: 7 |
Subjective pain measured by NRS (0-10) | 1.85 (1.70) |
Weight (kg) | 64.35 (12.07) |
Height (cm) | 164.16 (0.29) |
BMI (kg/m2) | 22.5 (2.89) |
Gender ratio (Female: Male ) | 22:12 |
Table 1: Baseline characteristics.
Shapiro-Wilks’ test and Q-Q plots indicated that the data did not conform to a normal distribution (p<0.05), consequently Spearman’s Correlation was applied to the data (r²=0.91, 95% CI 0.83 to 0.96, p<0.0001) indicating significant correlation between gamma oscillations and PPT (Figure 1).
Subsequent multiple regression test indicated PPT and NRS to be significant predictors of gamma variability (p<0.05). All other variables appeared to have no significant effect in the regression model (p>0.05) (Table 2).
Parameter | 95% CI | P-value |
---|---|---|
PPT | 3067 to 5205 | <0.0001 |
Subjective pain (NRS) | 587.9 to 2871 | 0.0024 |
Anxiety | -610.6 to 948.1 | 0.6600 |
Age | -97.59 to 240.1 | 0.3943 |
Weight | -117.8 to 156.8 | 0.7734 |
Height | -9745 to 1556 | 0.1487 |
Table 2: Interval correlation and P-values of variables from regression test.
Results from the Mann/Witney-U test to check for group differences in gamma levels between genders, indicated higher levels of gamma frequencies in females (p<0.05).
Discussion
The principal aim of this study was to determine whether there was a relationship between gamma oscillations and objective and subjective pain thresholds in an asymptomatic sample. The findings indicate that there is a strong correlation between gamma oscillations and PPT. Few studies have investigated the direct relationship between gamma oscillations and objective pain but available evidence indicates reduction in pain when partaking in activities that increase gamma oscillation. Meditation has shown effectiveness for chronic neck pain and headache frequency significantly reducing symptoms compared to controls [22,23]. Various studies have indicated that meditation increases gamma brainwaves [16]; there is no correlation between high gamma and meditation in this current study which may have been attributable to the small numbers practising this approach.
Gamma oscillations have been associated with working memory and verbal and non-verbal cognition tasks [24,25]. Therefore the state of mind of the cohort must also be considered, all of which were students experiencing the rigours of an undergraduate programme. Strong religious beliefs and praying has been implicated with decreasing pain [26]. Religious participants were able to reduce pain sensation during prayer and it has been indicated that prayer may play a role in pain management [27,28]. Religion is an artefact of complex interactions amid cultural systems and basic human brain function. Muslims after prayer were shown to have increased gamma oscillations, likewise Carmelite nun’s gamma oscillations also increased during prayer [29,30]. These cognitive processes seem to rely on the neural mechanism of descending pain inhibition in which gamma oscillations appear to play a key role. The religious beliefs and adherence to faith were not explored in the current study’s sample.
A cognitive distraction task was used whilst subjects received intermittent painful thermal stimuli. The more cognitively demanding the interference task, the more the pain intensity scores significantly reduced, suggesting that cognitive processes may modify pain [31]. High working memory load influences the neuronal response to painful stimulation in the dorsal horn of the corresponding spinal segment [32], which may provide explanation for why art, light and music therapy are perceived to be effective [33]. Findings also indicate that high gamma oscillations assist in the successful implementation of working memory [25]. Gamma oscillations have been associated with not only memory but perhaps a fundamental role allowing it to sub-serve to numerous cognitive functions [34]. Gamma oscillations’ role in cognitive processes may allow for direct targeting with potential modulation of pain coming from a gamma-effective strategy.
Emotions have been shown to potentially play an important role in pain, with positive emotions decreasing pain and shifting attention away from pain [35,36]. Participants that undertake social interactions and received support compared to those alone reported less pain [37]. Gamma oscillation appears to be enhanced during emotional situations [38,39]. Buddhist meditators’ gamma oscillations are reported to increase further when mediating on loving-kindness and compassion [40]. Gamma oscillations also appear to be enhanced when normal participants process emotional stimuli [41]. The emotional state of the individuals in this current study was not screened and may have had some influence on their responsiveness.
It is suggested that PPT decreases with age, [42] however age was not shown to be a confounding variable and had no implication on the correlation in this present study. This was possibly attributable to the narrow age range encountered. Soetanto et al. [43] suggested that men tolerate more pain than women. Women appeared to have a higher PPT in this study compared to men with females being the predominant gender in this study. A study by Racine et al. [44] conversely rejects the notion that there is a pattern in sex differences in pain sensitivity. There have been proposals that there were differences in responses to pain by different ethnic groups, this data was not collected in this study and should be assessed in further studies [45].
A comorbidity of depression is often pain, 65% of depressed patients present with one or more pain symptoms and 5-85% of patients with pain will be suffering from depression [46]. Although it would appear that pain and depression commonly exist together, their dynamic interactions and underlying mechanisms are largely unknown [47]. However patients suffering from depression appear to have significantly lower gamma oscillations [48]. The reduction in gamma oscillations in depression may be the underlying neural mechanism in the comorbidity being pain. In this study data about depression was not captured and it would be suggested that further research capture such characteristics to establish a link between gamma oscillations, depression and PPT.
A constant flow of sensory information means that a number of events compete for neural representation and perception, needing to optimise the utilization of cerebral processing resources [7]. Mechanisms of processing sensory information include gamma oscillations, representing modulation of neural oscillations [7,49,50]. Gamma oscillations have also been witnessed in response to painful stimuli [51,52]. The relationship between induced gamma oscillations and the processing of sensory stimuli indicate that these responses may not only be associated to the physical stimulus attributes, but also connected particularly to the subjectively weighted percept of a sensory event [7].
MyndPlay was initiated to provide brain training through entertainment using brainwave EEG technology. The EEG devise was designed to allow the viewer not only to have fun but to also develop control over their own emotions. This aimed to be a platform for alternative therapy by making mental training techniques available to a wider audience. The purpose was to create video simulations in which you progressively add controlled emotional, tension and stress through gaming mechanics. Gamma Oscillations elevated during motor imagery based on a video game. Parallel to this a further study suggested that Gamma Oscillations were significantly increased during high intensity events during game-play [53]. There is potential for EEG interfaces such as the MyndPlay device to influence attention and relaxation levels as a mechanism to manage chronic pain.
Study Limitations
Whist this study has shown a significant correlation between gamma oscillations and PPT, there are limitations to the validity of the results. Due to sample restrictions within the student cohort the results cannot be representative of the general population. It is also of note that the majority of participants’ data was gathered during the exam period which may have affected cognitive processing and potentially increased gamma oscillations [33].
The Myndplay Brainband XL device has insufficient research on the reliability and accuracy and therefore is considered a limiting factor despite its flexibility for monitoring outside of a laboratory environment. Although the same treatment room was used, controlling the external environmental noise is a factor that should be considered as gamma activity has been shown to increase during auditory processing [54].
Further Research
A symptomatic population would be recommended as well as collecting further baseline characteristics such as race, adherence to faith, exercise and a more in depth analysis of their meditation practice. A psychometric test to attempt to objectively measure aspects of mental ability would also be a useful tool to see if there is a correlation of gamma oscillations and PPT.
As multiple studies have suggested variables such as emotion, cognition and working memory can affect the spiking activities of gamma oscillations. Therefore future research may identify true gamma oscillations by applying further applicable statistics to attain periodicity [55-57]. Future work needs to discriminate between genuine pain-related oscillations and an increase resulting from greater spiking activity [58-63].
Development of long term studies looking at the effect of increasing gamma oscillation with participants that suffer with chronic pain is necessary [64-72]. Targeting gamma modification with a gaming strategy may offer a patient-centred alternative to the management of pain. This could be further explored through the use of online health communities and social networks in order to provide a platform for chronic pain sufferers to validate their experiences [72-82].
Conclusion
There is a statistically significant association between high gamma oscillations and PPT. This may represent a step forward in identifying a possible approach to influence pain via other neural mechanisms. Identifying that gamma oscillations may have an important role in PPT allows holistic therapies such as meditation, art, light and music therapy to have a scientific rationale behind their treatment as they all induce gamma oscillations. Strategies to manipulate gamma oscillations could be a fundamental process in assisting people in coping with chronic pain conditions.
References
- Ahmad AH, Aziz CB. The brain in pain. Malays J Med Sci. 2014;21: 46-54.
- Dowman R. Neural mechanisms underlying pain’s ability to reorient attention: evidence for sensitization of somatic threat detectors. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14(2): 805-17.
- Schulz E, May ES, Postorino M, et al. Prefrontal gamma oscillations encode tonic pain in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2015;1-8.
- Zhang ZG, Hu L, Hung YS, et al. Gamma-Band oscillations in the primary somatosensory cortex—A direct and obligatory correlate of subjective pain intensity. Journal of Neurosci. 2012;32(22): 7429-38.
- Merskey H, Bogduk N. IASP task force on taxonomy pain terms: A current list with definitions and notes on usage. Classification of Chronic Pain. 2. IASP Press, Seattle. 1994;210.
- Carlsson K, Andersson J, Petrovic P, et al. Predictability modulates the affective and sensory-discriminative neural processing of pain. NeuroImage. 2006;32(4): 1804-14.
- Gross J, Schnitzler A, Timmermann L, et al. Gamma oscillations in human primary somatosensory cortex reflect pain perception. PLOS Biology. 2007;5(5).
- Rossiter HE, Worthen SF, Witton C, et al. Gamma oscillatory amplitude encodes stimulus intensity in primary somatosensory cortex. Front Hum Neuroscience. 2013;7(362): 1-7.
- Naro A, Leo A, Bramanti P, et al. Moving toward conscious pain processing detection in chronic disorders of consciousness: Anterior cingulate cortex neuromodulation. J Pain. 2015;16(10): 1022-31.
- Tiemann L, May ES, Postorino M, et al. Differential neurophysiological correlates of bottom-up and top-down modulations of pain: Pain. 2015;156: 289-96.
- Tiemann L, Schulz E, Gross J, et al. Gamma oscillations as a neuronal correlate of the attentional effects of pain. Pain. 2010;150(2): 302-08.
- Lakatos P, Karmos G, Mehta AD, et al. Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science. 2008; 320(5872):110-13.
- Noton D. Migraine and photic stimulation : Report on a survey of migraineurs using flickering light therapy. Complement Therapy Nurse Midwifery. 2000;6(3): 138-42.
- Wang J, Wang J, Xing GG, et al. Enhanced gamma oscillatory activity in rats with chronic inflammatory pain. Front Neurosci. 2016;10(489).
- Li L, Liu X, Cai C, et al. Changes of gamma-band oscillatory activity to tonic muscle pain. Neuroscience Letters. 2016; 627:126-31.
- Hauswald A, Übelacker T, Leske S, et al. What it means to be Zen: Marked modulations of local and interareal synchronization during open monitoring meditation. NeuroImage. 2015;108: 265-73.
- Bhattacharya J, Petsche H. Universality in the brain while listening to music. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 2001;268(1484): 2423-33.
- Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Large EW, et al. Beta and gamma rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169(1): 89-92.
- Ziedan F, Martucci K, Kraft R, et al. Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain and mindfulness meditation. J Neurosci. 2011;31(14): 5540-48.
- Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Eslinger PJ, et al. The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. J Neurosci. 2002;22(7): 2730-36.
- Bijur PE, Latimer CT, Gallagher EJ. Validation of a verbally administered numerical rating scale of acute pain for use in the emergency department. Acad Emerg Med. 2003;10(4): 390-92.
- Jeitler M, Brunnhuber S, Meier L, et al. Effectiveness of jyoti meditation for patients with chronic neck pain and psychological distress — A randomized controlled clinical trial. J Pain. 2015;16(1): 77-86.
- Wachholtz AB, Malone CD, Pargament KI. Effect of different meditation types on migraine headache medication use. Behav Med. 2015;11: 1-8.
- Kahana MJ. The cognitive correlates of human brain oscillations. J Neurosci. 2006;26(6): 1669-72.
- Yamamoto J, Suh J, Takeuchi D, et al. Successful execution of working memory linked to synchronized high-frequency gamma oscillations. Cell. 2014;157(4): 845-57.
- De Villiers M, Maree JE, Van Belkum C. The Influence of chronic pain on the daily lives of underprivileged south africans. Pain Manag Nurs. 2015;16(2): 96-104.
- Dezutter J, Wachholtz A, Corveleyn J. Prayer and pain: The mediating role of positive re-appraisal. J Behav Med. 2011;34(6): 542-49.
- Jegindo EME, Vase L, Skewes JC, et al. Expectations contribute to reduced pain levels during prayer in highly religious participants. J Behav Med. 2013;36(4): 413-26.
- Beauregard M, Paquette V. EEG activity in cCarmelite nuns during a mystical experience. Neuroscience Letters. 2008;444(1): 1-4.
- Ridzwan MW, Fatihilkamal MW, Mahmood NH, et al. Salat and brainwave signal analysis. J Teknologi.2011;54(1): 181-92.
- Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, et al. Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain. 2002;125(2): 310-19.
- Sprenger C, Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, et al. Attention modulates spinal cord responses to pain. Current Biology. 2012;22(11): 1019-22.
- Jensen O, Kaiser J, Lachaux J, et al. Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30(7): 317-24.
- Fries P. Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32(1): 209-24.
- Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe KM. Emotional control of nociceptive reactions (ECON): Do affective valence and arousal play a role? Pain. 2007;136(2008): 250-61.
- Lumley MA, Cohen JL, Borszcz GS, et al. Pain and Emotion: A biopsychosocial review of recent research. J Clin Psychol. 2011;67(9): 942-68.
- Brown J, Sheffield D, Leary M, et al.Social supports and experimental pain. Psychosomatic medicine. 2003;659(2): 276-83.
- Headley DB, Paré D. In sync: Gamma oscillations and emotional memory. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7(170): 1-12.
- Onton J, Makeig S. High-frequency broadband modulations of electroencephalographic spectra. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3(61): 1-18.
- Lutz A, Greischar LL, Rawlings NB, et al. Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. PNAS. 2004;101(46): 16369-73.
- Keil A, Müller MM, Gruber T, et al. Effects of emotional arousal in the cerebral hemispheres: A study of oscillatory brain activity and event-related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(11): 2057-68.
- Lautenbacher S, Kunz M, Strate P, et al. Age effects on pain thresholds, temporal summation and spatial summation of heat and pressure pain. Pain. 2005;115(3): 410-18.
- Soetanto ALF, Chung JWY, Wong TKS. Are there gender differences in pain perception? J Neurosci nurs. 2016;38(3): 172-76.
- Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, et al. A systematic literature review of 10 years of research on sex/gender and experimental pain perception – Part 1: Are there really differences between women and men? Pain. 2012;153(3): 602-18.
- Rahim-Williams FB, Riley III JL, Herrera D. Ethnic identity predicts experimental pain sensitivity in African Americans and Hispanics. Pain. 2007;129(1-2): 177-84.
- Bair MJ, Robinson RL, Katon W, et al. Depression and pain comorbidity: A literature review. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163(2): 2433-45.
- Li JX. Pain and depression comorbidity: A preclinical perspective. Behav Brain Res. 2015;1276: 92-98.
- Canali P. Shared reduction of oscillatory natural frequencies in bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder and schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. 2015;184: 111-15.
- Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE. Et al. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science.2001;291(5508): 1560-63.
- Engel AK, Singer W. Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5(1): 16-25.
- Schulz E, Tiemann L, Schuster T,et al. Neurophysiological coding of traits and states in the perception of pain. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21: 2408‐14.
- Schulz E, Zherdin A, Tiemann L,et al. Decoding an individual’s sensitivity to pain from the multivariate analysis of EEG data. Cereb Cortex. 2011;22(5): 1118-23.
- McMahan T, Parberry I, Parsons TD. Modality specific assessment of video game player’s experience using the emotiv. Entertainment Computing. 2015;7: 1-6.
- Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W. Human gamma-band activity: A window to cognitive processing. Neuroreport. 2005;16(3): 207-11.
- Muresan RC, Jurjut OF, Moca VV, et al. The oscillation score: an efficient method for estimating oscillation strength in neuronal activity. J Neurophysio. 2008;99(3): 1333-53.
- Burns SP, Xing D, Shapley RM. Is gamma-band activity in the local field potential of V1 cortex a “clock” or filtered noise?. J Neurosci. 2011;31(26): 9658-64.
- Ray S, Maunsell JH. Network rhythms influence the relationship between spike-triggered local field potential and functional connectivity. J Neurosci. 2011;31(35): 12674-82.
- Jarvis MR, Mitra PP. Sampling properties of the spectrum and coherency of sequences of action potentials. Neural Computation. 2001;13(4): 717-49.
- Crone NE, Sinai A, Korzeniewska A. High-frequency gamma oscillations and human brain mapping with electrocorticography. Prog Brain Res. 2006;159: 275-95.
- Montgomery SM, Sirota A, Buzsáki G. Theta and gamma coordination of hippocampal networks during waking and rapid eye movement sleep. J Neurosci. 2008;28(26): 6731-41.
- Whittingstall K, Logothetis NK. Frequency-band coupling in surface EEG reflects spiking activity in monkey visual cortex. Neuron.2009;64(2): 281-89.
- Quilichini P, Sirota A, Buzsáki G. Intrinsic circuit organization and theta–gamma oscillation dynamics in the entorhinal cortex of the rat. J Neurosci. 2010;30(33): 11128-42.
- Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R. Cross-frequency phase–phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2012;32(2): 423-35.
- Choy EHS. The role of sleep in pain and fibromyalgia. Nat Rev Rheumatol. 2015;11(9): 513-20.
- Dawson R. How significant is a boxplot outlier? J Stat Educ. 2011;19(2): 1-13.
- Glauser ESD, Scherer KR. Neuronal processes involved in subjective feeling emergence: Oscillatory activity during an emotional monitoring task. Brain Topogr. 2008;20(4): 224-31.
- Guétin S, Giniès P, Siou DKA. The effects of music intervention in the management of chronic pain: A single-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Clin J Pain. 2012;28(4): 329-37.
- Gupta SK. The relevance of confidence interval and P-value in inferential statistics. Indian J Pharmacol. 2012;44(1): 143-44.
- Kothari DJ, Davis MC, Yeung EW, et al. Positive affect and pain: mediators of the within-day relation linking sleep quality to activity interference in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2015;156(3): 540-46.
- Kruk KA, Aravich PF, Deaver SP, et al. Comparison of brain activity during drawing and clay sculpting: A preliminary QEEG Study. Art Therapy. 2014;31(2): 52-60.
- Lacourt TE, Houtveen JH, Van Doomen LJP. Experimental pressure-pain assessments: Test–retest reliability, convergence
- Li XM, Yan H, Zhou KN, et al. Effects of music therapy on pain among female breast cancer patients after radical mastectomy: results from a randomized controlled trial. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2011;128(2): 411-19.
- McBeth J, Wilkie R, Bedson J, et al. Sleep disturbance and chronic widespread pain. Curr Rheumatol Rep. 2015;17(1): 1-10.
- Pain from the multivariate analysis of EEG data. Cereb Cortex (in press).
- Paola C, Sarasso S. Shared reduction of oscillatory natural frequencies in bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder and schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. 2015;184: 111-15.
- Roehrs T, Hyde M, Blaisdell, B. Sleep loss and REM sleep loss are hyperalgesic. Sleep. 2006;(29): 145-51.
- Roehrs TA, Harris E, Randall S, et al. Pain sensitivity and recovery from mild chronic sleep loss. Sleep. 2012;35(12): 1667-72.
- Russell J. Art therapy on a hospital burn unit: a step towards healing and recovery. Art Therapy. 1995;12(1): 39-45.
- Senkowski D, Kautz J, Hauck M, et al. Emotional facial expressions modulate pain-induced beta and gamma oscillations in sensorimotor cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31(41): 14542-50.
- Sepulveda MLA, Alonao J, Guevara M. Increased prefrontal-parietal EEG gamma band correlation during motor imagery in expert video game players. Research Gate. 2014;28(117): 26-35.
- Takeuchi S, Mima T, Murai R, et al. Gamma oscillations and their cross-frequency coupling in the primate hippocampus during sleep. SLEEP. 2015;38(7): 1085-91.
- Trauger-Querry B, Haghighi KR. Balancing the focus: art and music therapy for pain control and symptom management in hospice care. Hospice. 1999;14(1): 25-38.