Research Article - Research and Reports on Genetics (2018) Volume 2, Issue 2
Inheritance of qualitative traits in two populations of Nigerian local chicken ecotypes.
Gwaza DS*, Dim NI, Momoh OM
Department of Animal Breeding and Physiology, University of Agriculture, Makurdi, Nigeria
- *Corresponding Author:
- Gwaza DS
Department of Animal Breeding and Physiology
University of Agriculture
Nigeria
Tel: +23408036808979
E-mail: gwazadeeve@yahoo.com
Accepted Date: May 08, 2018
Citation: Gwaza DS, Dim NI, Momoh OM. Inheritance of qualitative traits in two populations of Nigerian local chicken ecotypes. J Res Rep Genet. 2018;2(2):22-28
Abstract
The study was conducted at akpehe poultry farm, Makurdi. Nigeria. A total of seven hundred day old chicks obtained from the mattings of one hundred dams mated to twenty sires were used for the study. The study was designed to provide information on the inheritance of qualitative traits in two populations of the local chicken ecotypes of Nigeria. White shank and beak, white and black skin, black and brown earlobes, white, yellow and brown eye colors fitted closely to the expected mendelian ratios of 3:1 as their alleles shows independent assortment and were thus heritable. Other qualitative traits did not fit into the expected mendelian ratios, indicating that multiple alleles may be involves in their expressions. Plumage color variations all deviated from their expected ratios. This may be due to multiple alleles or polygenic influence, involving gene interactions, gene complexes as well as genetically controlled events, and as such would not be heritable. Artificial selection must be applied to large variation in plumage colors to ensure there availability for adaptation for future use.
Keywords
Gene-complexes, Local-chicken-ecotype, Inheritance, Polygenic, Qualitative-trait.
Introduction
Phenotypic and genotypic characterization of indigenous chicken populations provides essential information to make rational decisions for improvement and development of effective breeding programmes. The report of Daguma [1] indicated that for African countries to improve indigenous poultry resources to meet their human needs, depends on their ability to identify the within and between variation existing among their chicken ecotypes. In Nigeria, work has been carried out on indigenous chicken [2-9]. There is however no report on the genetic inheritance of qualitative traits between ecotypes of indigenous chickens of Nigeria, a vital information required for the appropriate characterization, selection and identification of morphological variations. Thus, characterization of qualitative traits for both local adaptation with emphasis on morphological selection of traits that enhance adaptation are yet to be feasible with the Nigerian indigenous chicken. The potential of information on morphological variation within and between ecotypes that could identify capacities for adaptation for future use are not attainable. It is expected that natural and direct selection, and mutation may lead to nonrandom or directional changes in the allelic distribution and frequencies regulating qualitative traits. Thus, within and between ecotypes variation should occur, defining unique set of genes for special utility to environmental challenges [1]. Where diverse agro climates, enormous migration, natural and or man-made challenges existed, these are adequate reason for the substantial variation of various qualitative traits in the ecotypes of the indigenous chicken populations. The degree of genetic uniqueness and adaptive capacities of indigenous chicken can be assessed through phenotypic characterization.
The genetics of qualitative traits has been reported to be complex by Hutt [10]. According to the author, all the comb types, pea, walnut, rose, single and duplex are controlled by four pairs of genes; (RR, PP, rr, pp) the rose comb type (RrpP or RRPP), pea comb type (rrPp) [10]. The author also reported that single comb type (rrpp) is a simple homozygous recessive to rose and pea comb types. Walnut comb types (RrPp, RRPP) and duplex comb type (dd) are recessive. Hutt [10] reported that walnut comb type is controlled by gene complementarity or gene modifiers. Daguma [1] reported that skin coloration in birds is due to presence or absence of carotenoids. The authors observed that carotenoid are taken up from the circulation in both genotypes but are degraded by Beta-carotene deoxygenase 2 in the skin from birds carrying the white skin alleles (W*W)’ thereby producing a white skin. The authors also observed that yellow skin is caused by recessive allele (W*Y) that allow deposition of carotenoids in the skin. Daguma [1] also reported that aside the activities of the recessive gene, yellow skin is facilitated by one or more cis acting and tissue specific regulatory mutation(s) that inhibit the expression of beta– carotene deoxygenase 2 (BCDO2) in the skin. Shank colour is controlled by genes that affect the skin at varying depths. The visible color was due to the combined effect of the different colours of the dermis and the epidermis. So shank colour is a combination of upper skin and deeper skin pigmentation. Other genes can modify shank colour.
Schwanyana et al. and Oluyemi and Roberts [11,12] also reported red and white earlobe colours with red been the most common. The relative abundance of the red earlobe determining gene (I) differed significantly (P<0.05) from that of the white earlobe determining gene (i), and it apparently had a higher transmission potential. Thus the dominant gene is usually transmitted to the next generation more frequently while the recessive allele are transmitted less frequently and also due to mutations. The white earlobe could be due to the purine pigment which is controlled by a number of genes. The red earlobe colour could be due to the absence of the genes that invoke the purine pigmentation. The brown and black earlobe colours may be due to tinge on the white earlobe. Social preference and natural selection did favor the red earlobe colour hence its preponderance. Eye colours had been reported to be due to carotenoid pigments and blood supply of the iris. Brown eye colour was due to Er (birchen) and fibro melanotic gene (extended black, E) characterized by heavy eumelanin deposits throughout the eye in the absence of other melanin inhibitors. The white eye colour was due to the inhibitor of shank dermal melanin, Id, which also inhibits eye pigmentation [13].
Variations in melanin pigmentation of the skin and its epidermal integuments have long been reported [13]. Studies have also been carried out on the inheritance of melanization especially of the feathers, although the genetic bases for a number of phenotypes have not been determined [13]. Smith [13] also noted that genetics of melanization is complex, and also involves genetic interactions. These complexities are demonstrated between individual plumage colour genes, and certain gene complexes that are yet to be worked out. Smith [13] also observed that a number of different genetic routes produces identical or nearly identical phenotypes. Smith [13] also observed that physiological systems that are relatively unaffected by environmental components, controlled by a number of identifiable qualitative inherited genes, polygenic modifying complexes and the various interactions between these produces the final plumage coloration observed. The author also considered plumage colour as a polygenic trait and a simplified genetic model for other quantitatively inherited traits. While qualitative traits do not affect economic traits of farm animals directly, they played significant roles that enhances the adaptation and fitness of farm animals to their environment. The inheritance and distribution of qualitative traits becomes very critical in the subsequent generations of farm animals especially under the impact of climate change as they affect adaptation. There are reports of associations between qualitative and quantitative traits of farm animals. Gene interactions, modifiers and complexes controlling qualitative traits especially plumage colours and body pigmentation have associations with performance traits. The objective of this study was to provide information on the inheritance of qualitative traits of the local chicken ecotypes of Nigeria.
Materials and Methods
The study was conducted at akpehe poultry farm, Makurdi. Nigeria. A total of seven hundred day old chicks obtained from the mattings of one hundred dams mated to twenty sires were used for the study. The birds were given routine medication, water and feeds were provided adlibitum.
Data collection and analysis
Qualitative traits were measured as the ratio of the frequency of each trait in the offspring population to the number of observations of the trait in the parental population. Chi-square was used to evaluate the deviation of the occurrence of qualitative traits in the offspring population from their expectations in the same population.
Results
Inheritance of beak color
The Chi Square (χ2) test applied to the three classes of beak color in the Tiv and the Fulani ecotypes expected in the mendelian radio of 3:1 were highly significant (P>0.05) different from the expected (Table 1).
Table 1: χ2 on the three classes of beak colour with expected ratio of 3:1.
Trait Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. (0) | Expeted No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 | (0-e)2 E |
χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beak Tiv Colour | White | 54 | 65 | -11 | 121 | 1.86 | Ns |
Black | 91 | 65 | 26 | 676 | 10.4 | * | |
Brown | 115 | 195 | -80 | 6400 | 32.82 | * | |
Total | 260 | 325 | -65 | 7197 | 45.08 | ||
Beak Colour Fulani | White | 51 | 65 | -14 | 196 | 3.02 | Ns |
Black | 95 | 65 | 30 | 900 | 13.85 | * | |
Brown | 114 | 195 | 81 | 6561 | 33.65 | * | |
Total | 260 | 325 | -65 | 7657 | 5052 |
*Significant at (P<0.05).
χ2 =Chi square.
Ns=not significant.
Inheritance of skin and comb color
There were also significant (P>0.05) differences in the expected Mendelian ratio of 3:1 (Tables 2 and 3) in the Chi Square test on one class of the skin and comb colour of the Tiv and the Fulani ecotypes.
Table 2: χ2 on one class of skin colour with expected ratio of 3.1.
Trait/ Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 E |
χ2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Skin Colour Fulani | White | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 | * |
Total | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | |||
Skin Colour Tiv | White | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 | * |
Totals | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
χ2 =Chi square.
Table 3: χ2 on one class of comb colour with expected ratio of 3.1.
Trait/ Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 E |
χ2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Comb Colour Fulani | Red | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 | * |
Total | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | |||
Comb Colour Tiv | Red | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 | * |
Totals | 260 | 195 | 65 | 4225 | 21.67 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
χ2 =Chi square.
Inheritance of shank color
The χ2 test on the three classes of shank colours observed did not agree with the ratio of 3:1 and was significant (P>0.05) (Table 4). In the Fulani ecotype, the χ2 test applied to the two classes of shank color observed in the Mendelian ratio of 3:1 was not significantly (P>0.05) different from the expected.
Table 4: χ2 on two classes of shank colour with expected ratio of 3.1.
Trait/Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 | (0-e)2 E |
χ2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Shank colour Fulani |
White | 182 | 195 | -13 | 169 | 0.87 | Ns | |
Black | 78 | 65 | +13 | 169 | 2.6 | Ns | ||
Total | 260 | 260 | 0.0 | 338 | 2.93 | |||
Shank colour | White | 94 | 65 | 29 | 84.1 | 12.94 | * | |
Tiv | Green | 33 | 65 | -32 | 1024 | 1573 | * | |
Black | 133 | 195 | -62 | 3844 | 19.71 | * | ||
Total | 260 | 325 | 65 | 5707 | 48.38 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
Ns=not significant.
χ2Chi square
Inheritance of eye color
The χ2 test on the three classes of eye colors mendelian ratio of 1:2:1 were not significantly (P>0.05) different from the expected in the Tiv and Fulani ecotype (Table 5).
Table 5: χ2on three classes of eye colour with expected ratio of 3:1.
Triat/Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 | (0-e)2 e |
χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Eye colour Tiv | Yellow | 132 | 195 | -63 | 3969 | 20.35 | * |
White | 52 | 65 | -13 | 2.6 | Ns | ||
Brown | 76 | 65 | 11 | 169 | 1.86 | Ns | |
Total | 260 | 325 | -65 | 1242591 | 24.81 | ||
Eye colour Fulani | Yellow | 200 | 195 | 05 | 25 | 0.13 | Ns |
Brown | 60 | 65 | -05 | 25 | 0.39 | Ns | |
Total | 260 | 260 | 0.0 | 50 | 0.52 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
Ns=not significant.
χ2=Chi square.
Inheritance of earlobe color
The χ2 test on the three classes of earlobe colours observed showed that they were significantly (P>0.05) different from the expected ratio of 3:1 (Table 6) in the two ecotypes.
Table 6: χ2 on three classes of earlobe colour with expected ratio of 1:2:1.
Triat/Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No.(e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e) | (0-e)2 e |
χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Earlobe colour Fulani | Black | 58 | 65 | -7 | 49 | 0.75 | ns |
White | 117 | 65 | -13 | 169 | 2.6 | * | |
Brown | 85 | 65 | +20 | 400 | 6.15 | * | |
Total | 260 | 325 | - 65 | 6533 | 38.1 | ||
Earlobe colour Tiv | Black | 79 | 65 | 14 | 196 | 3.0154 | ns |
White | 30 | 65 | -35 | 1225 | 18.8462 | * | |
Brown | 103 | 65 | -05 | 25 | 0.39 | * | |
Red | 48 | 65 | -17 | 289 | 4.4462 | Ns | |
Total | 260 | 390 | -130 | 10174 | 69.7178 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
Ns=not significant
χ2=Chi square.
Inheritance of comb type
The χ2 test on the three classes of comb types were significantly (P>0.05) different from the expected 3:1 ratio (Table 7).
Table 7: χ2on three classes of Comb types with expected ratio of 3.1.
Trait/ Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (o-e) | (o-e)2 | (0-e)2/e | χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Comb type Fulani | Single | 258 | 195 | 63 | 3986 | 20.3539 | * |
Walnut | 01 | 65 | -64 | 4096 | 63.0154 | * | |
Rose | 01 | 65 | -64 | 4096 | 63.0154 | * | |
Total | 260 | 325 | 12161 | 146.3847 | |||
Comb type Tiv | Single | 259 | 65 | 194 | 37636 | 579.01 | * |
Walnut | 33 | 65 | -32 | 1024 | 15.73 | * | |
Total |
*Significant at (P<0.01)
χ2 =Chi Square.
Inheritance of plumage color
The χ2 test on the nine classes of plumage colors observed in the Tiv ecotype showed significant (P>0.05) difference from the expected 3:1 ratio (Table 8). In the same vein, 2 test on the ten classes of plumage colors in the Fulani ecotype showed significant (P>0.05) difference from the expected 3:1 ratio (Table 9).
Table 8: Chi Square (χ2) test on nine classes of plumage colours with expected ratio of 3:1.
Trait/ Ecotype |
Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 | (0-e)2 e |
χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Plumage Colour | |||||||
Tiv | Silver-brown | 15 | 65 | -50 | 2500 | 38.46 | * |
Mottle brown | 50 | 195 | -145 | 21025 | 107.82 | * | |
Blackish- brown | 60 | 195 | -135 | 18225 | 93.46 | * | |
Mottled | 05 | 65 | -60 | 3600 | 53.38 | * | |
Solid brown | 30 | 65 | -35 | 1225 | 18.85 | * | |
Solid black | 30 | 65 | -35 | 1225 | 18.85 | * | |
Mixed grey | 10 | 65 | -55 | 3025 | 46.54 | * | |
Light brown | 20 | 65 | -45 | 2025 | 31.15 | * | |
White mottle | 10 | 65 | -55 | 3025 | 46.54 | * | |
Total | 260 | 845 | 620 | 55875 | 455.05 | * |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
χ2=Chi square.
Table 9: Chi square (χ2) test applied to nine classes of plumage colours with expected ratio of 3:1.
Trait/Ecotype | Phenotypes | Obs. No. (0) | Expected No. (e) | Deviation (0-e) | (0-e)2 | (0-e)2 E |
χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Plumage Colour | |||||||
Fulani | White mottle | 25 | 65 | - 40 | 1600 | 24.62 | * |
Blackish- brown | 45 | 195 | -150 | 22500 | 115.39 | * | |
Solid black | 15 | 65 | -50 | 2500 | 38.41 | * | |
Mottle brown | 35 | 195 | -160 | 25600 | 131.28 | * | |
Light brown | 15 | 65 | -50 | 2500 | 38.46 | * | |
Dull brown | 07 | 65 | -58 | 3364 | 51.75 | * | |
Solid white | 08 | 65 | -57 | 3249 | 49.99 | * | |
Solid brown | 45 | 195 | -150 | 22500 | 115.39 | * | |
Light black | 35 | 195 | -60 | 25600 | 131.28 | * | |
Black mottle | 30 | 65 | -35 | 1225 | 18.85 | * | |
Total | 260 | 1170 | -910 | 110638 | 715.47 |
*Significant at (P<0.05)
χ2=Chi square.
Discussion
Segregation of genes for bird’s qualitative traits
The observed numbers in the different classes that fitted closely to the expected implied that such traits are monohybrid and their alleles showed independent assortment; hence the various descriptors of such characteristics are true to type in occurrence. Those observed numbers that did not fit into the expected indicated that, either the genetic model postulated to explain the result was wrong or multiple alleles were involved at a locus, between loci, or there were some form of relationship between the alleles determining the traits. Some of those identified, showed simple dominance, co-dominance, complementary effect or incomplete dominance. This could be possible reason why the observed ratios could not fit the expected values. For such traits, the best estimate of the true ratios of genotype groups is the observed ratios of the totals obtained from heterogeneous populations [14].
Skin colors
One skin color, white was observed in this study. Saidu [15] also observed that white skinned birds dominated the local chicken population in Bauchi State, Nigeria. The frequency of the white skin determining gene (W) did not differ significantly from the frequency of the gene controlling yellow skin. Mancha [8] also reported non significant difference between the abundance of the white skin determining gene (W) and the yellow skin determining gene (w) in Plateau state, Nigeria. He reported that skin coloration in birds is due to presence or absence of carotenoids. The authors observed that carotenoid are taken up from circulation in both genotypes (white and yellow skin colored) but are degraded by Beta-carotene deoxygenase 2 in the skin from birds carrying the white skin allele (W*W)’ thereby producing a white skin. The authors also observed that yellow skin is caused by a recessive allele (w*Y) that allows deposition of carotenoids in the skin. He also reported that aside the activities of the recessive gene, yellow skin is facilitated by one or more cis-acting and tissue specific regulatory mutation(s) that inhibit the expression of beta–carotene deoxygense 2 (BCDO2) in the skin. The wide preponderance of the white skin colour indicated that the white skin allele (W*W) had higher transmission potentials compared to the yellow skin allele (w*Y), natural selection and social preference (adaptation) seemed to have favored the white skin genotype, hence its wide preponderance.
Eye colors
Three eye colors, yellow, white and brown were observed in this study. At the W locus, two eye colors, yellow (w) and white (W) while at the E locus, the brown eye color (E) was observed. Meyer; Saidu; Mancha [8,15,16] reported similar observations. The high frequency of the yellow eye color determining gene again agreed with the highest yellow eye colour phenotype observed within the ecotypes. This was also true for the brown eye gene and its phenotype within the ecotypes. Again social preference and natural selection seemed to have favored the yellow eye color genotype. The yellow eye color was due to carotenoid pigments and blood supply of the iris. Brown eye color was due to Er (birchen) and fibro melanotic gene (extended black, E) characterized by heavy eumelanin deposits throughout the eye in the absence of other melanin inhibitors. The white eye colour was due to the inhibitor of shank dermal melanin, Id, which also inhibits eye pigmentation.
Comb colors
One comb color, red was noted in this study. Schwanyana et al. and Mancha [8,11] also reported red comb color to be the most commonest at Apae and kumi district in Uganda, and Jos in Plateau state, in Nigeria respectively. The frequency of the gene determining red comb color (R) was high, it transmitting potentials was equally high. These related positively to the high phenotypic frequency of red comb color in each population of the ecotypes.
Shank colors comb type
The estimated gene frequencies obtained in this study for comb types have been reported by Ikeobi et al. [17] who reported estimates of r=0.99, p=0.98, R=0.01 and P=0.02 in local chickens of South Western Nigeria. The lower frequencies of the dominant allele (P and R) obtained in this study indicated that the recessive alleles have higher transmission potential as noted in this study within and between the Fulani and Tiv local chicken ecotypes of the derived guinea savannah zone of Nigeria. The low frequency of R (0.03) and P (0.03) between the ecotypes observed in this study with a seemingly lower transmission potential of (0.007 and 0.01) and (0.003 and 0.00) for the P and R genes, in the Fulani and the Tiv ecotypes respectively. The insignificant occurrence of the Rose and Walnut comb type was justified. Social preference, natural selection and adaptation would favor single comb type since single large comb would be important in ensuring the survival and production performance of birds. At the R locus, one Wattle color, red, was observed in this study.
There were four shank colors observed in this study. At the W locus, two shank colors, White (W) and yellow (w) were noticed. While at the E locus, black shank color E was observed, and the interaction between W and E loci produced the green shank color (WE) Mbap and Zakar [4] Oluyemi and Roberts; Smith [18,19] also reported four shank colors (white, yellow, black and grey) in populations of local chicken in Yobe State, Nigeria and in warm wet climate respectively. The high frequency of the white shank determining gene (W) in the Fulani ecotype related positively to the high frequency of the white shank phenotype. This was also true for the occurrence of the black shank phonotype among the Tiv ecotype. Yellow and green shanks due to their low frequency of occurrence, was low in the population. In all, the white shank color appeared to be favored by social preference and natural selection. The preponderance of the black shank phenotype in the Tiv ecotype was due to their interaction with their environment. Shank colour is controlled by genes that affect the skin at varying depths. The visible color was due to the combined effect of the different colors of the dermis and the epidermis. So shank color is a combination of upper skin and deeper skin pigmentation. Other genes can modify shank colors. Yellow shanks are due to presence of lipochrome pigments when melanin is absent. They also noted that the intensity of the yellow color depends on the amount of exathophyll in the ration. These authors also noted that black shank is due to the presence of melanin pigment in the dermis, and when the melanin occurs in the epidermis, the shank color is greenish in appearance. When both melanin and lipochrome pigment are absent the shank is white
Earlobe color
At the 1 locus, two earlobe color, red and white, were observed in this study. The red earlobe color birds were more common. Schwanyana et al. and Oluyemi and Roberts [11,12] also reported red and white earlobe colors with red been the most common. The relative abundance of the red earlobe determining gene (I) differed significantly (P<0.05) from that of the white earlobe determining gene (i), and it apparently had a higher transmission potential. Thus the dominant gene is usually transmitted to the next generation more frequently while the recessive allele are transmitted less frequently and also due to mutations. The white earlobe could be due to the purine pigment which is controlled by a number of genes. The red earlobe color could be due to the absence of the genes that invoke the purine pigmentation. The brown and black earlobe colors may be due to tinge on the white earlobe. Social preference and natural selection did favor the red earlobe color hence its preponderance. Two earlobe color, red and white, were observed in this study. The red earlobe color birds were more common. Schwanyana et al. and Oluyemi and Roberts [11,12] also reported red and white earlobe colors with red been the most common. The relative abundance of the red earlobe determining gene (I) differed significantly (P<0.05) from that of the white earlobe determining gene (i), and it apparently had a higher transmission potential. Thus the dominant gene is usually transmitted to the next generation more frequently while the recessive allele are transmitted less frequently and also due to mutations. The white earlobe could be due to the purine pigment which is controlled by a number of genes. The red earlobe color could be due to the absence of the genes that invoke the purine pigmentation. The brown and black earlobe colors may be due to tinge on the white earlobe. Social preference and natural selection did favour the red earlobe color hence its preponderance.
Beak colors
The chicken unlike mammals has no teeth. The mouth is in the form of a beak, which is thick and pointed. Obioha [20] noted that the beak is used for obtaining food, building nest, turning eggs, caring for chicks and preening feathers. He reported that on the dorsal surface of the tail of most birds is a small oil gland (uropygixal gland or oil preen gland) which supplies oil for keeping the feathers glossy and waterproof and prevent the beak from becoming brittle. Lucas and Stettenheim [21] reported that the yellow color of the beak is due to the presence of xanthophyll’s. He reported that three types of beak colors were observed in Southern Plateaus Local Government Areas of Nigeria. This author observed that the brown beak chickens accounted for 43.09 percent were the most common followed by the black beaked (30.7%) while the yellow beaked (26.21%), which were the least common.
Plumage color variations
The significant deviations of the observed from the expected ratios of the various plumage colors was related to the various genetic mechanisms regulating plumage colors in chickens. The genetics of plumage color through melanization is complex, and also involves genetic interactions. These complexities are demonstrated between individual plumage color genes, and certain gene complexes that are yet to be worked out. Smith [13] also observed that a number of different genetic routes produces identical or nearly identical plumage phenotypes. Smith [13] also observed that physiological systems that are relatively unaffected by environmental components, controlled by a number of identifiable qualitative inherited genes, polygenic modifying complexes and the various interactions between these produces the final plumage colouration observed. He reported that although both melanin and carotenoid pigments contribute to feather color of certain avian species, it is the melanins that determines plumage color and patterns. The presence and distribution of the melanins result in differences in the feather forms within age and sex, as well as structural variations between and within other feather traits. Solid colors of black dominant, recessive white, albinism, depigmentation, silver, multiple colors of different grades and gold. These colors were not pale and do to not give the appearance of the diluting effect of genes. There were also diluted colors, though uniform, appear pale, suggestive of weakened action of genes responsible for their expression. The color extension and restriction where, even though color was present, it was not uniformly distributed to all parts. Instead of causing the distribution of color throughout the entire plumage or among regions, color distribution occurred within feathers resulting in patterns. This could only be possible if each plumage color was controlled by series of genes, genetic complexes or genetically controlled events. Oluyenu and Roberts [12,18] also reported that each plumage color and pattern is the result of a series of genetically determined events. The regulation of plumage colors and patterns through interaction effects of genes, gene-complexes and genetically controlled events, indicated that although plumage color is a qualitative traits, it is control by polygenic effects. The segregation of the alleles, their distribution and inheritance cannot fit into the expected Mendelian ratio of 3:1. The transfer of parental plumage colors to their offspring’s in local chicken populations would be low due to the polygenic nature of the genetic mechanisms controlling plumage colours [22-24].
Plumage color however plays important role in body heat regulation and eventual adaptation of local chicken ecotypes to their environment. Selection of suitable plumage color for unique environmental needs is desirable for continued adaptation to environmental challenges from generation to generation especially in the phase of climate change impacts. The capacities of natural, unconscious and counter selections are inadequate to preserve potential plumage colors to enhance future adaptation to unique environmental needs of the future. Artificial selection can be applied to large variations in plumage colors for each geographical location to accumulate geographical zone-specific local chicken ecotypes.
Conclusion and Recommendation
Conclusion
White shank and beak, white and black skin, black and brown earlobes, white, yellow and brown eye colors fitted closely to the expected mendelian ratio of 3:1 indicating that these traits are monohybrid and heritable. Their alleles showed independent assortment. The other qualitative traits that differs significantly from the expected indicated that multiple alleles were involved. The deviation of plumage color traits from the expected was due the influence of polygenic influence of genes on the trait. Plumage color traits were controlled by series of genes, genetic interactions and gene-complexes as well as genetically controlled events and as such would not be heritable. The capacities of natural, unconscious and counter selections are inadequate to preserve potential qualitative traits to enhance future adaptation to unique environmental needs of the future.
Recommendation
Selection of suitable plumage color and other qualitative traits for unique environmental needs is desirable for continued adaptation to environmental challenges from generation to generation. Artificial selection can be applied to large variations in plumage colors for each geographical location to develop geographical zone-specific local chicken ecotypes.
References
- Daguma R. Phenotypic characterization of some indigenous chicken ecotypes of Ethiopia. Livest Res Rural Dev. 2006;18(9).
- Nwosu CC, Omeje SSI. Improved annual egg production from Nigerian local chicken by gold-line cross progeny. Nigerian J Anim Prod. 1985;12(1&2):161-4.
- Nwosu CC. Review of indigenous poultry Research. In: Sonaiya, E.B. Rural Poultry in Africa; Proceeding of an International Workshop held on 13th-16th November, Ileife, Nigeria. 1990;6.
- Mbap ST, Zakar H. Characterization of local chickens in Yobe State Nigeria. In: The role of agriculture in poverty alleviation. Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Agricultural Society of Nigeria. 2000;126-31.
- Adedokun SD, Sonaiya EB. Comparisons of the performance of Nigerian chickens from three agro-ecological zones. Livest Res Rural Dev. 2001;13(2).
- Ojo V. Incidence and Influence of Naked neck and frizzle genes on body size of local genes. M.Sc. Thesis, University of Ilorin. Nigeria. 2002.
- Adesina OM. Gene frequency and influence of polydactyl and Ptylopody on body size of Nigerian local chickens. M.Sc thesis University of Ilorin Nigeria. 2002.
- Mancha YP. Characterization of Local Chickens in Northern part of the Jos Plateau. Ph.D Thesis Animal Production Programme, School of Agriculture, Abubakar Tafawa Belewa University Bauchi. 2004.
- Fayeye TR, Oketoyin AB. Characterization of the Fulani Chicken ecotypes for thermoregulatory feather gene. Livestock Research for Rural Development. 2006;18(3).
- Hutt FB. Genetics of the fowl. McGraw-Hill Book Company Inc, New York. 1964.
- Schwanyana E, Onyait AO, Ogwal J, et al. Characteristics of Rural Chicken Production in Apal and Kumi Districts of Uganda. Paper presented at NARO Scientific Conference, Kampala, Uganda. 2001;5-10.
- Oluyemi JA, Roberts FC. Poultry production in warm wet climates. Macmillan Press Ltd, London. 1979;198.
- Smith JR. Genetic bases for plumage colour pattern in the new Hamsphire fowl. J Heredity. 1970;61:280-3.
- Little MT, Hills FJ. Agricultural experimentation, Design and analysis. John Willy and Sons. New York. Chickester, Brisbane, Toronto, Singapore. 1982.
- Saidu IA. Characterization of local chicken in Bauchi: PGD Dissertation, Animal Production Programme, Abubakar Tafawa Balewa University, Bauchi. 2001.
- Meyer DB. The avian eye. In: Avian Physiology. 4th Edition springer verlag, New York. 1987.
- Ikeobi CON, Ozoje MO, Adebambo OA, et al. Frequencies of Feet Feathering and Comb type genes in the Nigerian Local Chicken. Partanika J Trop Sci. 2001;24(2):147-50.
- Oluyemi JA, Roberts FA. Poultry Production. Spectrum Books Ltd, Ibadan. 2000.
- Smith AJ. Poultry. Ed. Revised edition. Macmillan Education Limited. 2001;1-63.
- Obioha EC. A guide to poultry production in the tropics. Accna publishers, Enugu.1992;212.
- Lucas AM, Stettenheim PR. Avian Anatomy, Integument. Part H. Agricultural Handbook 362. U.S. Government Printing Office. Washington DC. 1972.
- Morejohn GV. Plumage colour allelism in the red jungle fowl (Gallus gallus) and related domestic forms. Genet.1965;38:519-30.
- Nwosu CC, Gowen FA, Obiaha FC, et al. A biometrical study of the conformation of the native chickens. Nigerian J Anim Prod. 1985;12:141-6.
- Omeje SSI, Nwosu CC, Umunna CU. Heritability and component of variance estimates of egg weight and shell thickness in local and Yaffa x Gold-link crossbred chickens. Nigeria Agr J. 1987;22:110-117.