Journal of Psychology and Cognition

All submissions of the EM system will be redirected to Online Manuscript Submission System. Authors are requested to submit articles directly to Online Manuscript Submission System of respective journal.
Reach Us +441518081136

Short Communication - Journal of Psychology and Cognition (2022) Volume 7, Issue 8

Evidence from grapheme-colour synaesthesia for the numerical abilities of young synaesthetes.

Louisa Rinaldi*

Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, England, United Kingdom

*Corresponding Author:
Louisa Rinaldi
Department of Psychology
University of Sussex
England, United Kingdom
E-mail: L.Rinaldi@susse.ac.uk

Received: 22-July-2022, Manuscript No. AAJPC-22-74911; Editor assigned: 25-July-2022; PreQC NO. AAJPC-22-74911 (PQ); Reviewed: 9-August-2022, QC No. AAJPC-22-74911; Revised: 16-August-2022, Manuscript No. AAJPC-22-74911 (R); Published: 23-August-2022, DOI: 10.35841/aajpc-7.8.139

Citation: Rinaldi L. Evidence from grapheme-colour synaesthesia for the numerical abilities of young synaesthetes. J Psychol Cognition. 2022;7(8):139

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Psychology and Cognition

Abstract

Synaesthesia is a condition in which a mixing of the senses occurs; for example, sounds trigger the experience of colour. Previous reports suggest this may be familial, but no systematic studies exist. In addition, there are no reliable prevalence or sex-ratio figures for the condition, which is essential for establishing if the reported sex ratio (female bias) is reliable, and if this implicates a sex-linked genetic mechanism. Two independent population studies were conducted in the city of Cambridge, England (studies 1 and 2 here), as necessary background to the family genetic study of synaesthesia (study 3). Studies 1 and 2 arrived at an almost identical prevalence rate for synaesthesia: approximately 1 case in 2000. The sex ratio found was 6:1 (female: male). A third of cases also reported familial aggregation. In study 3 six families were examined, and first-degree relatives were tested for genuineness of the condition. All six families were indeed multiplex for synaesthesia. Alternative modes of inheritance are discussed. Keywords: Synaesthesia, Inheritance, Senses, Hippocampus, Metaphors.

Keywords

Synaesthesia, Inheritance, Senses, Hippocampus, Metaphors.

Introduction

Synaesthesia has been described as a perceptual phenomenon that creates a ‘merging of senses’. Therefore, academic treatments have focused primarily on its sensory characteristics and similarities with veridical perception. This approach has dominated, despite parallel work that has suggested conceptual influences are involved, including data that show a large number of synaesthetic variants are triggered by linguistic symbols (e.g. words). These variants are the focus of a novel subfield that applies psycholinguistic methodology to the study of linguistic synaesthesias. This approach is redefining notions of synaesthesia and of the interplay between perceptual and non-perceptual systems, in addition to informing general theories of language. This review examines the emergent field of linguistic synaesthesia research and the broad range of linguistic mechanisms that are implicated [1].

Current research in neurology posits relationships between the hippocampus, the neurological condition known as synaesthesia, and the associations made in the construction of synaesthetic metaphors (see, for example, Cytowic 1993). My research addresses the following questions: Are there similarities in the sensory ranking patterns of synaesthetic associations and synaesthetic metaphors?; and, if so, What apropose that, with the information gleaned from current research in neurology, a new interpretation of synaesthesia and synaesthetic metaphors -- one which views synaesthetic metaphors as culturally and linguistically shaped, but with some neurological underpinnings might be obtained. This view allows for cross-linguistic studies to explore and separate the layers of semantic trends and cognitive processes, and reveals that non-linguistic methods might be used to distinguish the innate trends [2].

Can conclude into synaesthesia actually benefit cognitive science?

People with synaesthesia show an enhanced memory relative to demographically matched controls. The most obvious explanation for this is that the ‘extra’ perceptual experiences lead to richer encoding and retrieval opportunities of stimuli which induce synaesthesia (typically verbal stimuli). Although there is some evidence for this, it is unlikely to be the whole explanation. For instance, not all stimuli which trigger synaesthesia are better remembered (e.g., digit span) and some stimuli which do not trigger synaesthesia are better remembered. In fact, synaesthetes tend to have better visual memory than verbal memory. We suggest that enhanced memory in synaesthesia is linked to wider changes in cognitive systems at the interface of perception and memory and link this to recent findings in the neuroscience of memory [3].

This suggests that synaesthesia uses the same (or an analogous) mechanism of exogenous cross-modal orienting as normal perception. Overall, the results support the conclusion that this form of synaesthesia recruits some of the same mechanisms used in normal cross-modal perception rather than using direct, privileged pathways between unimodal auditory and unimodal visual areas that are absent in most other adults [4]. A neurological condition known as grapheme-colour synaesthesia results in permanent colour associations with letters and numerals. Studies on synaesthesia have revealed distinctions between synaesthetes and non-synaesthetes that go beyond the phenomenon of synaesthesia itself (e.g., differences in their cognition, personality, and creativity). Little is known about the characteristics of young synaesthetes because this study has largely been limited to adult synaesthetes. Although differences in mathematical skill are unclear, most data point to benefits. While some studies demonstrate benefits others point to challenges. In this study, we examined the numerical cognition of a sizable sample of kids who had grapheme-color synaesthesia [5].

References

  1. Rinaldi LJ, Smees R, Carmichael DA, et al. Numeracy skills in child synaesthetes: Evidence from grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cortex. 2020;126:141-52.
  2. Indexed at, Google Scholar, Cross Ref

  3. Hughes JE, Gruffydd E, Simner J, et al. Synaesthetes show advantages in savant skill acquisition: training calendar calculation in sequence-space synaesthesia. Cortex. 2019;113:67-82.
  4. Indexed at, Google Scholar, Cross Ref

  5. Havlik AM, Carmichael DA, Simner J. Do sequence-space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills? Yes, but there are individual differences. Cogn Process. 2015;16(3):245-53.
  6. Indexed at, Google Scholar, Cross Ref

  7. Uno K, Yokosawa K. Apparent physical brightness of graphemes is altered by their synaesthetic colour in grapheme-colour synaesthetes. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):1-9.
  8. Indexed at, Google Scholar, Cross Ref

  9. Carmichael DA, Smees R, Shillcock RC, et al. Is there a burden attached to synaesthesia? Health screening of synaesthetes in the general population. Br J Psychol. 2019;110(3):530-48.
  10. Indexed at, Google Scholar, Cross Ref

Get the App